Evolución de la investigación sobre la cognición canina. Una revisión sistemática utilizando la teoría de grafos

Contenido principal del artículo

Maria Cristina Correa-Duque
Anyerson Stiths Gómez-Tabares

Resumen

El objetivo es realizar una revisión sistemática de la evolución de la investigación sobre la cognición canina, enfatizando en los dominios sociales. La búsqueda bibliográfica se efectuó en Scopus y Web of Science. Para el manejo de los datos se realizó un análisis de clusterización mediante teoría de grafos. Se empleó las herramientas Sci2 Tool y Gephi. Los resultados se segmentaron en estudios clásicos, estructurales y recientes. En estos estudios se encontró que los perros disponen de habilidades para el reconocimiento y el uso de formas humanas de comunicación que superarían a los lobos y primates no humanos. También, las habilidades comunicativas serían más similares entre perros y humanos que entre humanos y chimpancés. Estos hallazgos han sido interpretados bajo las hipótesis explicativas de la herencia evolutiva, de exposición y domesticación humanas, las cuales reconocen que el entorno social humano representa el nicho ecológico natural para esta especie.

Detalles del artículo

Cómo citar
Evolución de la investigación sobre la cognición canina. Una revisión sistemática utilizando la teoría de grafos. (2021). Revista Argentina De Ciencias Del Comportamiento, 13(3), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.32348/1852.4206.v13.n3.27516
Sección
Revisiones

Cómo citar

Evolución de la investigación sobre la cognición canina. Una revisión sistemática utilizando la teoría de grafos. (2021). Revista Argentina De Ciencias Del Comportamiento, 13(3), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.32348/1852.4206.v13.n3.27516

Referencias

Agnetta, B., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use. Animal Cognition, 3(2), 107–112. doi: 10.1007/s100710000070

Amon, M. J., & Favela, L. H. (2019). Distributed cognition criteria: Defined, operationalized, and applied to human-dog systems. Behavioural Processes, 162, 167-176. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2019.03.001

Baillargeon, R. (1999). Young infants’ expectations about hidden objects: a reply to three challenges. Developmental Science, 2(2), 115–132. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00061

Bastian, M., Heymann, S., & Jacomy, M. (2009). Gephi: An Open Source Software for Exploring and Manipulating Networks. Proceedings of the International AAAI Conference on Web and Social Media, 3(1), 361-362.

Belger, J., & Bräuer, J. (2018). Metacognition in dogs: Do dogs know they could be wrong?. Learning & Behavior, 46(4), 398–413. doi: 10.3758/s13420-018-0367-5

Bentosela, M., Barrera, G., Jakovcevic, A., Elgier, A., & Mustaca A. (2008). Effect of reinforcement, reinforcer omission and extinction on a communicative response in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Behavioural Processes, 78(3), 464-469. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.03.004

Biondi, L. M., Bó, M. S., & Vassallo, A. I. (2008). Experimental assessment of problem solving by Milvago chimango (Aves: Falconiformes). Journal of Ethology, 26(1), 113-118. doi: 10.1007/s10164-007-0035-2.

Bräuer, J., & Call, J. (2011). The magic cup: Great apes and domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) individuate objects according to their properties. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125(3), 353–361. doi: 10.1037/a0023009

Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Visual perspective taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 88(3-4), 299-317. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.03.004.

Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Making inferences about the location of hidden food: Social dog, causal ape. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(1), 38–47. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.120.1.38

Brubaker, L., & Udell, M. A. R. (2018). The effects of past training, experience, and human behaviour on a dog’s persistence at an independent task. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 204, 101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.04.003

Budaev, S. (1998). How many dimensions are needed to describe temperament in animals: A factor reanalysis of two data sets. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 11, 17-29.

Bunford, N., Csibra, B., & Gácsi, M. (2019). Individual Differences in Response to Ambiguous Stimuli in a Modified Go/No-Go Paradigm are Associated with Personality in Family Dogs. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 11067. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-47510-z

Call, J., Braüer, J., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117(3), 257-263. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.3.257

Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes. En A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, & S. T. Parker (Eds.). Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 371-403). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Carballo, F., Freidin, E., & Bentosela, M. (2015). Estudios sobre cooperación en perros domésticos: una revisión crítica. Revista Colombiana de Psicología, 24(1), 145-163. doi: 10.15446/rcp.v24n1.41221

Carmona Cañabate, S. (2014). Cognición social. En D. Redolar Ripoll (Comp). Neurociencia cognitiva (pp. 693-716). Madrid: Medica Panamericana.

Chow, P. K. Y., Lea, S. E. G., & Leaver, L. A. (2016). How practice makes perfect: the role of persistence, flexibility and learning in problem-solving efficiency. Animal Behaviour, 112, 273–283. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.11.014

Cooper, J. J., Ashton, C., Bishop, S., West, R., Mills, D. S., & Young, R. J. (2003). Clever hounds: social cognition in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 81(3), 229–244. doi: 10.1016/s0168-1591(02)00284-8

D’Aniello, B., Scandurra, A., Prato-Previde, E., & Valsecchi, P. (2015). Gazing toward humans: A study on water rescue dogs using the impossible task paradigm. Behavioural Processes, 110, 68–73. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.022

Diverio, S., Menchetti, L., Riggio, G., Azzari, C., Iaboni, M., Zasso, R., … Santoro, M. M. (2017). Dogs’ coping styles and dog-handler relationships influence avalanche search team performance. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 191, 67–77. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.02.005

Duque, P., & Cervantes, L. S. (2019). Responsabilidad Social Universitaria: una revisión sistemática y análisis bibliométrico. Estudios Gerenciales, 35(153), 451-464. doi: 10.18046/j.estger.2019.153.3389

Elgier, A. M., Jakovcevic, A., Mustaca, A. E., & Bentosela, M. (2012). Pointing following in dogs: are simple or complex cognitive mechanisms involved? Animal Cognition, 15(6), 1111–1119. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0534-6.

Fox, M. W. (1971). Behaviour of wolves, dogs and related canids. London: Jonathan Cape.

Frank, H., & Frank, M. G. (1982a). On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior. Applied Animal Ethology, 8(6), 507–525. doi: 10.1016/0304-3762(82)90215-2

Frank, H., & Frank, M. G. (1982b). Comparison of problem-solving performance in six-week-old wolves and dogs. Animal Behaviour, 30(1), 95–98. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(82)80241-8

Gangopadhyay, N. (2017). The future of social cognition: paradigms, concepts, and experiments. Synthese, 194(3), 655-672. doi: 10.1007/s11229-016-1162-5.

Griffin, A. S., & Diquelou, M. C. (2015). Innovative problem solving in birds: a cross-species comparison of two highly successful passerines. Animal Behaviour, 100, 84-94. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.11.012

Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. (2002). The Domestication of Social Cognition in Dogs. Science, 298(5598), 1634–1636. doi: 10.1126/science.1072702

Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Communication of Food Location Between Human and Dog (Canis Familiaris). Evolution of Communication. An International Multidisciplinary Journal, 2(1), 137–159. doi: 10.1075/eoc.2.1.06har

Hare, B., Rosati, A., Kaminski, J., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). The domestication hypothesis for dogs’ skills with human communication: a response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne et al. (2008). Animal Behaviour, 79(2), e1–e6. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.031

Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (1999). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113(2), 173–177. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.113.2.173

Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68(3), 571-581. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.011

Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human like social skills in dogs?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439-443. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003

Hauser, M. D. (1996). The Evolution of Communication. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Hauser, M. D., Comins, J. A., Pytka, L. M., Cahill, D. P., & Velez-Calderon, S. (2011). What experimental experience affects dogs’ comprehension of human communicative actions?. Behavioural Processes, 86(1), 7-20. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.07.011

Herrmann, E., Hernandez-Lloreda, M. V., Call, J., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2010). The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychological Science, 21(1), 102–110. doi: 10.1177/0956797609356511

Horowitz, A. (2011). Theory of mind in dogs? Examining method and concept. Learning & Behavior, 39(4), 314–317. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0041-7

Horowitz, A. (2017). Smelling themselves: Dogs investigate their own odours longer when modified in an “olfactory mirror” test. Behavioural Processes, 143, 17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.08.001

Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Hutton, B., Catalá-López, F., & Moher, D. (2016). La extensión de la declaración PRISMA para revisiones sistemáticas que incorporan metaanálisis en red: PRISMA-NMA. Medicina Clínica, 147(6), 262–266. doi: 10.1016/j.medcli.2016.02.025

Ittyerah, M., & Gaunet, F. (2008). The response of guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis Familiaris) to cues of human referential communication (pointing and gaze). Animal Cognition, 12(2), 257–265. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0188-6

Jakovcevic, A., Mustaca, A., & Bentosela, M. (2012). Do more sociable dogs gaze longer to the human face than less sociable ones? Behavioural Processes, 90(2), 217–222. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.01.010

Jaro, M. A. (1989). Advances in Record-Linkage Methodology as Applied to Matching the 1985 Census of Tampa, Florida. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 84(406), 414-420. doi: 10.2307/2289924

Kaminski, J., Bräuer, J., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2009). Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human’s perspective. Behaviour, 146(7), 979–998. doi: 10.1163/156853908x395530

Kaminski, J., Neumann, M., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Dogs, Canis familiaris, communicate with humans to request but not to inform. Animal Behaviour, 82(4), 651–658. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.06.015

Kaminski, J., & Nitzschner, M. (2013). Do dogs get the point? A review of dog–human communication ability. Learning and Motivation, 44(4), 294–302. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2013.05.001

Kraus, C., Van Waveren, C., & Huebner, F. (2014). Distractible dogs, constant cats? A test of the distraction hypothesis in two domestic species. Animal Behaviour, 93, 173–181. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.04.026

Kubinyi, E., Virányi, Z., & Miklósi, Á. (2007). Comparative Social Cognition: From wolf and dog to humans. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 2, 26-46. doi: 10.3819/ccbr.2008.20002

Lazarowski, L., & Dorman, D. C. (2015). A comparison of pet and purpose-bred research dog (Canis familiaris) performance on human-guided object-choice tasks. Behavioural Processes, 110, 60-67. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.021

Lazarowski, L., Rogers, B., Waggoner, L. P., & Katz, J. S. (2019). When the nose knows: ontogenetic changes in detection dogs’ (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to social and olfactory cues. Animal Behaviour, 153, 61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.05.002

Lea, S. E. G., & Osthaus, B. (2018). In what sense are dogs special? Canine cognition in comparative context. Learning & Behavior, 46, 335–363. doi: 10.3758/s13420-018-0349-7

MacLean, E. L., & Hare, B. (2015). Dogs hijack the human bonding pathway. Science, 348(6232), 280-281. doi: 10.1126/science.aab1200

MacLean, E. L., Herrmann, E., Suchindran, S., & Hare, B. (2017). Individual differences in cooperative communicative skills are more similar between dogs and humans than chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 126, 41–51. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.01.005

McMahon, S., Macpherson, K., & Roberts, W. A. (2010). Dogs choose a human informant: Metacognition in canines. Behavioural Processes, 85(3), 293–298. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.07.014

Miklósi, A. (2009). Dog Behaviour, Evolution, and Cognition. United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.

Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, Z., & Csányi, V. (2003). A Simple Reason for a Big Difference: Wolves Do Not Look Back at Humans, but Dogs Do. Current Biology, 13(9), 763–766. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00263-x

Miklósi, Á., Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (1998). Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Animal Cognition, 1(2), 113–121. doi: 10.1007/s100710050016

Miklósi, Á., Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2000). Intentional behaviour in dog-human communication: an experimental analysis of “showing” behaviour in the dog. Animal Cognition, 3(3), 159–166. doi: 10.1007/s100710000072.

Miklósi, Á., & Soproni, K. (2006). A comparative analysis of animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture. Animal Cognition, 9(2), 81–93. doi: 10.1007/s10071-005-0008-1

Miklósi, A., & Topál, J. (2012). The Evolution of Canine Cognition. En J. Vonk & T. K. Shackelford (Ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 194-216). doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738182.013.0011

Miklósi, Á., & Topál, J. (2013). What does it take to become “best friends”? Evolutionary changes in canine social competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(6), 287-294. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.005.

Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2004). Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us? Animal Behaviour, 67(6), 995–1004. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.10.008

Miklósi, A., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2007). Big thoughts in small brains? Dogs as a model for understanding human social cognition. NeuroReport, 18(5), 467–471. doi: 10.1097/wnr.0b013e3280287aae

Mulcahy, N. J., & Hedge, V. (2012). Are great apes tested with an abject object-choice task? Animal Behaviour, 83(2), 313-321. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.019

Nagasawa, M., Murai, K., Mogi, K., & Kikusui, T. (2011). Dogs can discriminate human smiling faces from blank expressions. Animal Cognition, 14(4), 525-533. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0386-5

Osthaus, B., Lea, S. E. G., & Slater, A. M. (2005). Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) fail to show understanding of means-end connections in a string-pulling task. Animal Cognition, 8(1), 37–47. doi: 10.1007/s10071-004-0230-2

Pilley, J. W., & Reid, A. K. (2011). Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents. Behavioural Processes, 86(2), 184–195. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.11.007.

Povinelli, D. J., Bierschwale, D. T., & Cech, C. G. (1999). Comprehension of seeing as a referential act in young children, but not juvenile chimpanzees. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 37–60. doi: 10.1348/026151099165140

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515-526. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512

Range, F., & Virányi, Z. (2015). Tracking the evolutionary origins of dog-human cooperation: the “Canine Cooperation Hypothesis”. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1-10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01582

Robledo, S., Osorio, G., & López, C. (2014). Networking en pequeña empresa: una revisión bibliográfica utilizando la teoría de grafos. Revista Vínculos, 11(2), 6-16. doi: 10.14483/2322939X.9664

Sci2 Team. (2009). Science of Science (Sci2) Tool [software]. Indiana University and SciTech Strategies. Recuperado de: https://sci2.cns.iu.edu

Soproni, K., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2001). Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(2), 122–126. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.115.2.122

Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2010). What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions. Biological Reviews, 85(2), 327–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185x.2009.00104.x

Valencia-Hernández, D. S., Robledo, S., Pinilla, R., Duque-Méndez, N. D., & Olivar-Tost, G. (2020). SAP Algorithm for Citation Analysis: An improvement to Tree of Science. Ingeniería e Investigación, 40(1), 45-49. doi: 10.15446/ing.investig.v40n1.77718

Virányi, Z., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, A., & Csányi, V. (2004). Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’ attentional focus. Behavioural Processes, 66(2), 161-172. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2004.01.012

Virányi, Z., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V. (2006). A nonverbal test of knowledge attribution: a comparative study on dogs and children. Animal Cognition, 9(1), 13–26. doi: 10.1007/s10071-005-0257-z

Wallis, W. D. (2007). A Beginner’s Guide to Graph Theory (2 Ed.). Boston: Birkhauser.

Wang, S., Baillargeon, R., & Brueckner, L. (2004). Young infants’ reasoning about hidden objects: evidence from violation-of-expectation tasks with test trials only. Cognition, 93(3), 167–198. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.012

Wechsler, B. (1995). Coping and coping strategies: a behavioural view. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 43(2), 123–134. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(95)00557-9

Wobber, V., & Hare, B. (2009). Testing the social dog hypothesis: Are dogs also more skilled tan chimpanzees in non-communicative social tasks? Behavioural Processes, 81(3), 423-428. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.04.003

Wobber, V., Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Wrangham, R., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(3), 547–573. doi: 10.1002/dev.21125

Zuluaga, M., Robledo, S., Osorio- Zuluaga, G. A., Yathe, L., Gonzalez, D., & Taborda, G. (2016). Metabolómica y Pesticidas: Revisión sistemática de literatura usando teoría de grafos para el análisis de referencias. Nova, 14(25), 121–138. doi: 10.22490/24629448.1735