Expandiendo el espacio de búsqueda para una ciencia de la conciencia perceptual
Contenido principal del artículo
Resumen
Detalles del artículo
La RACC aplicará la licencia internacional de atribuciones comunes creativas (Reconocimiento 4.0 Internacional: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Bajo esta licencia, se permite cualquier explotación de la obra, incluyendo la explotación con fines comerciales y la creación de obras derivadas, la distribución de las cuales también está permitida sin ninguna restricción. Esta licencia es una licencia libre según la Freedom Defined. La única condición es que siempre y en todos los casos se cite a los autores y a la fuente original de publicación (i.e., RACC). Esta licencia fue desarrollada para facilitar el acceso abierto, gratuito y libre a trabajos originales científicos y artísticos.
Cómo citar
Referencias
Abarbanel, H. & Rabinovich, M. (2001). Neurodynamics: Nonlinear dynamics and neurobiology. Current Opinion in &eurobiology, 11, 432-430.
Adams, F. & Aizawa, K. (2008). Why the mind is still in the head. En Robbins, P. & Aydede, M. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition (pp. 78-95). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Anderson, M. (2003). Embodied cognition: A field guide. Artificial Intelligence, 149, 91-130.
Ballard, D. (1991). Animate vision. Artificial Intelligence Journal, 48, 57-86.
Ballard, D., Hayhoe, M., Pook, P. & Rao, R. (1997). Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 723-767.
Beer, R. (2000). Dynamical approaches in cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 91-99.
Bressler, S. (1996). Interareal synchronization in the visual cortex. Behavioural Brain Research, 76, 37-49.
Bressler, S. & Kelso, S. (2001). Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 26-36.
Chalmers, D. (2000). What is a neural correlate of consciousness. En Metzinger, T. (Ed.), &eural Correlates of Consciousness (pp. 17-39). Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Chemero, A. (2009). Radical Embodied Cognitive Science.Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Chiel, H. & Beer, R. (1997). The brain has a body. Trends in &eurosciences, 20, 553-557.
Churchland, P., Ramachandran, V. & Sejnowski, T. (1994). A critique of pure vision. En Koch, C. & Davis, J. (Eds.) Large-Scale &euronal Theories of the Brain (pp. 23-60). Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again. Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 345-351.
Crick, F. & Koch, C. (1990). Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in &euroscience, 2, 263-275.
Crick, F. & Koch, C. (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 97-107.
Crick, F. & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. &ature &euroscience, 6, 119-126.
Dennett, D. (1996). Contenido y Conciencia. Barcelona: Gedisa.
Dewey, J. (2006). Essays in Experimental Logic. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.
Engel, A., Fries, P. & Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. &ature Reviews &euroscience, 2, 704-716.
Engel, A., Friston, K., Kelso, S., König, P., Kovács, I., MacDonald III, A., Miller, E., Phillips, W., Silverstein, S., Tallon-Baudry, C., Triesch, J. & Uhlhaas, P. (2010). Coordination in behavior and cognition. En von der Malsburg, C., Phillips, W. & Singer, W. (Eds.),
Dynamic Coordination in the Brain: From &eurons to Mind (pp. 267-299). Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Fingelkurts, A. & Fingelkurts, A. (2004). Making complexity simpler. International Journal of &euroscience, 114, 843-862.
Freeman, W. & Holmes, M. (2005). Metastability, instability, and state transitions in neocortex. Neural Networks, 18, 497-504.
Freeman, W. & Skarda, C. (1985). Spatial EEG patterns, nonlinear dynamics and perception: The neoSherringtonian view. Brain Research Reviews, 10, 147-175.
Gazzaniga, M. (Ed.), (1995). The Cognitive &eurosciences. Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Gomila, A. & Calvo Garzón, F. (2008). Directions for an embodied cognitive science: towards an integrated approach. En Calvo Garzón, F. & Gomila, A. (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Science: An Embodied Approach (pp. 1-25). Oxford: Elsevier.
Haken, H. (2006). Synergetics of brain function. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 110-124.
Hardcastle, V. & Stewart, M. (2002). What do brain data really show? Philosophy of Science, 69, 72-82.
Hurley, S. (1998). Consciousness in Action. Cambridge, Ma.: Harvard University Press.
Hurley, S. & Noë, A. (2003). Neural plasticity and consciousness. Biology and Philosophy, 18, 131-168.
Jack, A. & Prinz, J. (2003). Searching for a scientific experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11, 51-56.
Järvilehto, T. (1998). Efferent influences on receptors in knowledge formation. Psycoloquy, 9, recuperado en: URL =<ftp://ftp.princeton.edu/pub/harnad/Psycoloquy/1998.volume.9>
Kelso, S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Kelso S. & Tognoli E. (2007). Toward a complementary neuroscience. En Perlovsky, L. & Kozma, R. (Eds.), Neurodynamics of Cognition and Consciousness (pp. 39-59). Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.
Kirsh, D. (1991). Today the earwig, tomorrow man. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 161-184.
Le Van Quyen, M. (2003). Disentangling the dynamic core. Biological Research, 36, 67-88.
Marr, D. (1985). Visión. Madrid: Alianza. McIntosh, A. (2000). Towards a network theory of cognition. Neural Networks, 13, 861-870.
Mossio, M. & Taraborelli, D. (2008). Action-dependent perceptual invariants: From ecological to sensorimotor approaches. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1324-1340.
Noë, A. (2004). Action in Perception. Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Noë, A. & Thompson, E. (2004a). Are there neural correlates of consciousness? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11, 3-28.
Noë, A. & Thompson, E. (2004b). Sorting out the neural basis of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11, 87-98.
O’Regan, K. & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 939-1031.
Pascal, F. & O’Regan, J. K. (2008). Commentary on Mossio and Taraborelli: Is the enactive approach really sensorimotor? Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1341-1342.
Pérez Velázquez, J. L. (2005). Brain, behaviour, and mathematics. Physica D, 212, 161-182.
Pessoa, L., Thompson, E. & Noë, A. (1998). Finding out about filling-in. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 723-802.
Prinz, J. (2000). A neurofunctional theory of visual consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 243-259.
Prinz, J. (2006). Putting the brakes on enactive perception. Psyche, 12. 1-19, recuperado en: URL =
Prinz, J. (2008). Is consciousness embodied? En Robbins, P. & Aydede, M. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition (pp. 419-436). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Prinz, J. & Barsalou, L. (2000). Steering a course for embodied representation. En Dietrich, E. & Markman, A. (Eds.), Cognitive Dynamics: Conceptual and representational change in humans and machines (pp. 23-61). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. (1997). The space around us. Science, 277, 190-191.
Rodriguez, E., George, N., Lachaux, J.-P., Martinerie, J., Renault, B. & Varela, F., (1999). Perception's shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity. Nature, 397, 430-433.
Ryle, G. (1952). The Concept of Mind. Londres: Hutchinson University Library.
Seth, A., Izhikevich, E., Reeke, G. & Edelman, G. (2006). Theories and measures of consciousness: An extended framework. Proceedings of the &ational Academy of Sciences, 103, 10799-10804.
Skarda, C. (1999). The perceptual form of life. En Núñez, R. & Freeman, W. (Eds.), Reclaiming Cognition: The Primacy of Action, Intention, and Emotion (pp. 79-93). Exeter: Short Run Press.
Skarda, C. & Freeman, W. (1987). How brain make chaos in order to make sense of the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 161-173.
Taraborelli, D. & Mossio, M. (2008). On the relation between the enactive and the sensorimotor approach to perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1343-1344.
Teller, D. & Pugh, E. (1983). Linking propositions in color vision. En Mollon, J. & Sharpe, L. (Eds.), Colour Vision: Physiology and Psychophysics (pp. 577-589). Londres: Academic Press.
Thompson, E. & Varela, F. (2001). Radical embodiment: Neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 418-425.
Uttal, W. (2003). The &ew Phrenology: The Limits of Localizing Cognitive Processes in the Brain. Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Van Gelder, T. & Port, R. (1995). It's about time. En Port, R. & van Gelder, T. (Eds.), Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition (pp. 1-43). Cambridge, Ma. / Londres: MIT Press.
Venturelli, N. (2008). La crítica anti-representacionalista en la ciencia cognitiva corporizada. En Faas, H & Severgnini, H. (Eds.), Epistemología e Historia de las Ciencias vol. 14 (pp. 549-556). Córdoba: Editorial Universitaria.
Venturelli, N. (2009). Heurísticas y niveles explicativos en el enfoque enactivo sobre la percepción. En Letzen, D. & Lodeyro, P. (Eds.), Epistemología e Historia de las Ciencias, vol. 15 (pp. 515-520). Córdoba: Editorial Universitaria.
Vera A. & Simon, H. (1993). Situated action: A symbolic interpretation. Cognitive Science, 17 (1), 7-48.
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 625-636.