Caracterización de los tipos de respuesta de parpadeo en un procedimiento de condicionamiento discriminativo en humanos
Main Article Content
Abstract
We implemented a human eyeblink conditioning procedure that allowed the characterization of their different responses: alpha, voluntary, conditioned, and unconditioned. For this, 8 participants received 66 trials of a tone (conditioned stimulus A +) paired with air-puff in the eye (unconditioned stimulus) and 66 trials of a vibration (conditioned stimulus B-). The results indicated differences in the frequency and temporal topography of the responses. Specifically, the alpha and unconditioned responses exhibited the lowest and highest amplitudes, respectively. In general, the conditioned and voluntary responses are similar in all measured parameters. The frequency analysis showed only evidence of discrimination in conditioned and voluntary responses with a higher response to stimulus A than stimulus B. We discuss the relevance of identifying and distinguishing these responses in any classical conditioning routine.
Article Details
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
La RACC aplicará la licencia internacional de atribuciones comunes creativas (Reconocimiento 4.0 Internacional: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Bajo esta licencia, se permite cualquier explotación de la obra, incluyendo la explotación con fines comerciales y la creación de obras derivadas, la distribución de las cuales también está permitida sin ninguna restricción. Esta licencia es una licencia libre según la Freedom Defined. La única condición es que siempre y en todos los casos se cite a los autores y a la fuente original de publicación (i.e., RACC). Esta licencia fue desarrollada para facilitar el acceso abierto, gratuito y libre a trabajos originales científicos y artísticos.
How to Cite
References
Bolaram, A., Coe, T. E., Power, J. M., & Cheng, D. T. (2020). Awareness and differential eyeblink conditioning: effects of manipulating auditory CS frequencies. Learning & Memory, 27(2), 78-82. doi: 10.1101/lm.050146.119
Brown, P., & Jenkins, H. (1968). Auto-shaping the pigeon’s key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11(1), 1-8. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1968.11-1
Chaplin, C. (1925). The Gold Rush [Película]. Estados Unidos: Charles Chaplin Productions.
Christian, K. M., & Thompson, R. F. (2003). Neural substrates of eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention. Learning & Memory, 10(6), 427-455. doi: 10.1101/lm.59603
Clark, R. E., Manns, J. R., & Squire, L. R. (2002). Classical conditioning, awareness, and brain systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(12), 524-531. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02041-7
Clark, R. E., & Squire, L. R. (1999). Human eyeblink classical conditioning: Effects of manipulating awareness of the stimulus contingencies. Psychological Science, 10(1), 14–18. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00099
Estes, W., & Skinner, B. (1941). Some quantitative properties of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29(5), 390-400. doi: 10.1037/h0062283
García, J., & Koelling, R. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4(1), 123-124. doi: 10.3758/BF03342209
Goodrich, K. (1966). Experimental analysis of response slopes and latency as criteria for characterizing voluntary and nonvoluntary responses in eyeblink conditioning. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80(14), 1-34. doi: 10.1037/h0093904
Gormezano, I. (1965). Yoked comparisons of classical and instrumental conditioning of the eyelid response: An addendum on “voluntary responders”. En W. F. Prokasy (Ed). Classical conditioning (pp. 47-70). New York: Appleton-Century-Croffts.
Gormezano, I., Schneiderman, N., Deux, E., & Fuentes, I. (1962). Nictitating membrane: Classical conditioning and extinction in the albino rabbit. Science, 138(3536), 33-34. doi: 10.1126/science.138.3536.33
Grant, D. A. (1945). A sensitized eyelid reaction related to the conditioned eyelid response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(5), 393–402. doi: 10.1037/h0057865
Grant, D. A., & Norris, E. B. (1947). Eyelid conditioning as influenced by the presence of sensitized Beta-responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37(5), 423–433. doi: 10.1037/h0054090
Grant, D., Norris, E., & Hornseth, J. (1948). Sensitization of the Beta response under verbally induced inhibitory set and counter-reinforcement. American Journal of Psychology, 61(1), 66-72. doi: 10.2307/1417293
Hilgard, E., & Campbell, A. (1936). The course of acquisition and retention of conditioned eyelid responses in man. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19(2), 227-247. doi: 10.1037/h0055600
Huang, H., Wu, B., Li, Q., Yao, J., Li, X., Yang, Y., Wu, G.-Y., & Sui, J. F. (2014). Awareness is essential for differential delay eyeblink conditioning with soft-tone but not loud-tone conditioned stimuli. Neuroscience Bulletin, 30(3), 433-440. doi: 10.1007/s12264-013-1400-5
Knuttinen, M.-G., Power, J. M., Preston, A. R., & Disterhoft, J. F. (2001). Awareness in classical differential eyeblink conditioning in young and aging humans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115(4), 747–757. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.4.747
Lovibond, P. F., Liu, J. C., Weidemann, G., & Mitchell, C. J. (2011). Awareness is necessary for differential trace and delay eyeblink conditioning in humans. Biological Psychology, 87(3), 393-400. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.05.002
Lovibond, P. F., & Shanks, D. R. (2002). The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: Empirical evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 28(1), 3–26. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.28.1.3
Manns, J. R., Clark, R. E., & Squire, L. R. (2000). Awareness predicts the magnitude of single‐cue trace eyeblink conditioning. Hippocampus, 10(2), 181-186. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(2000)10:2<181::AID-HIPO7>3.0.CO;2-V
Martin, I., & Levey, A. (1969). The genesis of the classical conditioned response. New York: Pergamon Press.
McLaren, I. P., Forrest, C. L. D., McLaren, R. P., Jones, F. W., Aitken, M. R. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2014). Associations and propositions: The case for a dual-process account of learning in humans. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 108, 185-195. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.09.014
Mitchell, C. J., De Houwer, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(2), 183–246. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X09000855
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rasmussen, A., & Jirenhed, D. A. (2017). Learning and timing of voluntary blink responses match eyeblink conditioning. Scientific Reports, 7, 3404. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-03343-2
Rescorla, R. A. (1967). Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review, 74(1), 71–80. doi: 10.1037/h0024109
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. En A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current theory and research (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton Century Crofts.
San Diego Instruments (equipos y software, 2022). Eyeblink Conditioning System. San Diego: San Diego Instruments.
Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (1994). What are the memory systems of 1994? En D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 1–38). Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Schade Powers, A., Coburn-Litvak, P., & Evinger, C. (2010). Conditioned eyelid movement is not a blink. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(2), 641–647. doi: 10.1152/jn.00631.2009
Shanks, D. R. (2010). Learning: From association to cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 61(1), 273-301. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100519
Spence, K. W., & Ross, L. E. (1959). A methodological study of the form and latency of eyelid responses in conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(5), 376–381. doi: 10.1037/h0045837
Squire, L. R. (1994). Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. En D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994 (pp. 203–231). Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Vogel, E., Brandon, S., & Wagner, A. (2003). Stimulus representation in SOP: - II. An application to inhibition of delay. Behavioural Processes, 62(1-3), 27-48. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00050-0.
Vogel, E., & Ponce, F. (2011). Empirical Issues and Theoretical Mechanisms of Pavlovian Conditioning. En E. Alonso & E. Mondragón (Eds.), Computational Neuroscience for Advancing Artificial Intelligence: Models, Methods and Applications (pp. 81-110). New York: Medical Information Science Reference IGI Global.
Vogel, E. H., Ponce, F. P., & Wagner, A. R. (2019). The development and present status of the SOP model of associative learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(2), 346-374. doi: 10.1177/1747021818777074
Wagner, A. R., & Vogel, E. H. (2008). Configural and elemental processing in associative learning: Commentary on Melchers, Shanks and Lachnit. Behavioural Processes, 77(3), 446-450. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2007.09.011
Weidemann, G., & Antees, C. (2012). Parallel acquisition of awareness and differential delay eyeblink conditioning. Learning & Memory, 19(5), 201-210. doi: 10.1101/lm.024851.111
Weidemann, G., Best, E., Lee, J. C., & Lovibond, P. F. (2013). The role of contingency awareness in single-cue human eyeblink conditioning. Learning & Memory, 20(7), 363-366. doi: 10.1101/lm.029975.112
Weidemann, G., Satkunarajah, M., & Lovibond, P. F. (2016). I think, therefore eyeblink: The importance of contingency awareness in conditioning. Psychological Science, 27(4), 467-475. doi: 10.1177/0956797615625973
Weidemann, G., Tangen, J. M., Lovibond, P. F., & Mitchell, C. J. (2009). Is Perruchet’s dissociation between eyeblink conditioned responding and outcome expectancy evidence for two learning systems? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35(2), 169–176. doi: 10.1037/a0013294